Who Blames the Victim?
IF
you are mugged on a midnight stroll through the park, some people will
feel compassion for you, while others will admonish you for being there
in the first place. If you are raped by an acquaintance after getting
drunk at a party, some will be moved by your misfortune, while others
will ask why you put yourself in such a situation.
What
determines whether someone feels sympathy or scorn for the victim of a
crime? Is it a function of political affiliation? Of gender? Of the
nature of the crime?
In a recent series of studies, we found that the critical factor lies in a particular set of moral values. Our findings, published on Thursday in the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
show that the more strongly you privilege loyalty, obedience and purity
— as opposed to values such as care and fairness — the more likely you
are to blame the victim.
In my article entitled Political Divide,
I introduced Jonathon Haidt’s work and the theoretical framework that
attempts to explain the current pervasive and seemingly intractable
political acrimony within the United States. Haidt and his colleagues
offer the Moral Foundations Theory, the
implications of which, suggest that this divide is a result of a moral
relativism of sorts – whereas one’s moral composition essentially drives
one’s political affiliation. Despite the perspective from each of the
polar extremes, individuals in the opposite group are not in fact
amoral, instead, Haidt et al., (2009) claim that they have different
valuations of five universal morals. According to Haidt, the five
universal morals include: (a) harm/care (strong empathy for those that
are suffering and care for the most vulnerable); (b)
fairness/reciprocity (life liberty and justice for all); (c)
ingroup/loyalty – (tribalism, patriotism, nationalism); (d)
authority/respect (“mechanisms for managing social rank, tempered by the
obligation of superiors to protect and provide for subordinates” Haidt,
2008); and (e) purity/sanctity (“related to the evolution of disgust,
that makes us see carnality as degrading and renunciation as noble”
Haidt, 2008)."
These
two sets of values have been the object of much scholarly attention.
Psychologists have found that when it comes to morality, some people
privilege promoting the care of others and preventing unfair behaviors.
These are “individualizing values,” as they can apply to any individual.
Other people privilege loyalty, obedience and purity. These are
“binding values,” as they promote the cohesion of your particular group
or clan.
Binding
and individualizing values are not mutually exclusive, and people have
varying degrees of both. But psychologists have discovered that the
extent to which you favor one relative to the other predicts various
things about you. For example, the more strongly you identify with
individualizing values, the more likely you are to be politically
progressive; the more strongly you identify with binding values, the
more likely you are to be politically conservative.
Our
animating insight was that these two clusters of values entail
different conceptions of victims. Proponents of individualizing values
tend to see a dyad of victim and perpetrator (a victim is hurt, a
perpetrator does the hurting). Proponents of binding values, however,
may see behaviors as immoral even when there is no obvious victim — for
example, the “impure” act of premarital sex or the “disloyal” act of
flag burning — and may even feel that doing the right thing sometimes
requires hurting others (as with honor killings, to pick an extreme
example). So we hypothesized that support for binding values would
correlate with a greater tendency to blame victims.
We
conducted several studies, involving 994 research participants. First
we examined how their moral values related to their tendency to
stigmatize victims versus to see victims as injured. We provided minimal
descriptions of victims of various crimes — rape and molestation,
stabbing and strangling — and asked the participants how much they
considered the victims as “injured” or “contaminated.”
While
we expected that all participants would be more likely to view
sexual-crime victims than non-sexual-crime victims as contaminated
(which is indeed what we found), we also found, surprisingly, that the
more strongly people endorsed binding values, the more strongly they
considered any victim to be contaminated — regardless of the nature of
the crime.
Furthermore,
the more people saw a victim as contaminated, the less they saw that
victim as injured. Throughout, we controlled for other variables and
found that it was moral values — binding values, in particular — and not
political orientation, gender or religiosity that determined the
results.
In
another study, participants read descriptions of specific cases of rape
and robbery and rated both the victim and the perpetrator on how
“responsible” they were for the outcome, as well as how much a change in
their actions could have changed things. We found that the more
strongly people endorsed binding values, the more they strongly they
attributed responsibility to victims and the more they saw victims’
behaviors as influencing the outcome. We found the opposite pattern for
people endorsing individualizing values.
Can
anything be done to change people’s perceptions of victims and
perpetrators? In another study, we explored whether nudging people to
focus on perpetrators versus victims could affect people’s moral
judgments. We did so by placing either the perpetrator or the victim in
the subject position in a majority of sentences in descriptions of
sexual assault (e.g., “Lisa was forced by Dan” versus “Dan forced
Lisa”). We then asked the participants to assign percentages of blame to
the victim and perpetrator.
Consistent
with our previous findings, the more participants endorsed binding
values, the more blame they assigned to victims and the less blame they
assigned to perpetrators. But we also found that focusing their
attention on the perpetrator led to reduced ratings of victim blame,
victim responsibility and references to victims’ actions, whereas a
focus on victims led to greater victim blaming. This was surprising: You
might assume that focusing on victims elicits more sympathy for them,
but our results suggest that it may have the opposite effect.
Victim
blaming appears to be deep-seated, rooted in core moral values, but
also somewhat malleable, susceptible to subtle changes in language. For
those looking to increase sympathy for victims, a practical first step
may be to change how we talk: Focusing less on victims and more on
perpetrators — “Why did he think he had license to rape?” rather than
“Imagine what she must be going through” — may be a more effective way
of serving justice.
Laura Niemi is a postdoctoral associate in psychology at Harvard. Liane Young is an associate professor of psychology at Boston College.
http://www.nytimes.com/2016/06/26/opinion/sunday/who-blames-the-victim.html?emc=edit_tnt_20160624&nlid=32999454&tntemail0=y
http://www.nytimes.com/2016/06/26/opinion/sunday/who-blames-the-victim.html?emc=edit_tnt_20160624&nlid=32999454&tntemail0=y
2 comments:
https://frumfollies.wordpress.com/2016/05/25/the-murder-of-chaim-weiss-is-there-a-child-sexual-abuse-angle/
Was child porn aficionado Waldman the molester in Long Beach Yeshiva referred to by Lopin?
Then in the comment by Jonathan, the cousin of Nechemya Weberman who allegedly molested his own nephew seems to be Beis Ruchel Williamsburg menahel Bentzion Feuerwerger.
Is Feuerwerger also from the Belsky side of the Weberman family?
http://nypost.com/2012/12/05/orthodox-pre-teen-allegedly-sexually-abused-drew-disapproval-from-sect-leadership-says-principal/
Feuerwerger, who is Weberman’s first cousin, drew incredulous laughter from several Orthodox spectators when he testified that the modesty squad — or Vaad Ha’Tnius — didn’t exist.
The alleged victim testified last week that part of the reason she was afraid to report Weberman was his Vaad Ha’Tnius membership.
http://nypost.com/2016/06/12/professor-who-donates-sperm-in-city-bathrooms-has-sired-22-kids/
Here's a candidate for the Board of YSV. This sick creep Ari Nagel who graduated Yeshiva of Spring Valley and Wolmark's Shaarei Torah has 22 kids, mostly a different color than he is.
http://theunorthodoxjew.blogspot.com/2012/08/he-calls-him-glatt.html
His father who is a big shot at Moses Tendler's shul is Torah Vodaas class of 1956 who remembers when Tress was molesting Pirchei boys at Agudah of Williamsburg.
Post a Comment